Cross-cultural psychology
Cross-cultural psychology is the scientific study of human behavior and cognitive processes across different cultures, emphasizing the similarities and differences that arise from cultural contexts. This field challenges traditional psychological assumptions that cognitive processes are universal, suggesting instead that culture profoundly shapes how individuals think and behave. Researchers like Richard Nisbett and Ara Norenzayan argue that fundamental aspects of thinking and perception are influenced by cultural factors, although some innate cognitive building blocks may constrain these cultural influences.
Culture encompasses shared characteristics within communities, including history, beliefs, and values, which can transcend generations. The way individuals perceive themselves can vary significantly between cultures; for instance, individualistic cultures prioritize personal independence, while collectivistic cultures emphasize group interdependence. This divergence affects various psychological phenomena, such as the fundamental attribution error, where individuals in different cultures explain behavior based on dispositional or situational factors differently.
Additionally, cultural variations extend to family structures, with practices like polygamy often emerging in response to environmental and economic needs. Overall, cross-cultural psychology highlights the intricate interplay between culture and cognition, encouraging a deeper understanding of human behavior through a diverse lens.
Cross-cultural psychology
- TYPE OF PSYCHOLOGY: Biological bases of behavior; cognition; developmental psychology; multicultural psychology
SIGNIFICANCE: Traditional views of psychology often ignored the role of culture in characterizing human behavior and thought. Researchers have since begun to appreciate the role of culture in psychology and recognize that culture and thought are often mutually interdependent.
Introduction
Cross-cultural psychology is a broad term for the scientific study of human behavior and mental or cognitive processes within different cultures. In general, this field addresses similarities and differences between cultures. According to the American social psychologists Richard Nisbett and Ara Norenzayan, the view that there are differences between cultures, at least in terms of cognitive processes, was not widely held in the twentieth century. Instead, most psychologists assumed that basic cognitive processes were universal—that the fundamental aspects of thinking and perceiving that involve attention, memory, learning, and reasoning operated in the same way among all cultures. Based on their research and that of other scholars, Nisbett and Norenzayan concluded that the basic processes of thinking and behavior are shaped by culture, although there are aspects of thinking and perceiving that may be innategenetic or possessed at birthand that limit or constrain the degree to which such shaping is possible.
![Picture of Jefferson Fish. CC-BY-SA-3.0 (creativecommons.org/licenses/by-sa/3.0)], via Wikimedia Commons 93871872-60274.jpg](https://imageserver.ebscohost.com/img/embimages/ers/sp/embedded/93871872-60274.jpg?ephost1=dGJyMNHX8kSepq84xNvgOLCmsE2epq5Srqa4SK6WxWXS)
What is Culture?
Culture describes general characteristics shared by a community of people. This community can be bound together by factors such as history, familial relationships, locales, music, cuisine, religious beliefs, and values. Culture includes expression of these traits within the community, as well as to outsiders. These values may transcend generations and can be instilled through different mediums such as oral histories, song, and written scripts.
How Thinking Constrains Culture
In the field of developmental psychology, there is much evidence to suggest that very young children have sets of basic building blocks that they use to understand human minds, important entities, and world events. These sets of building blocks are thought to be innate and domain specific. For example, one set helps children understand how other people think, and another set helps them understand the properties of objects. Evidently, they are common to all infants across cultures and limit the types of thinking about the world that can exist in any culture.
For example, the American developmental psychologist Elizabeth Spelke describes an experiment in which infants were shown a single toy animal placed on a stage. A curtain was lowered to hide or occlude the toy, and a second toy was shown to the infants and then placed behind the screen. Next, the screen was raised, revealing either both toys or only one of the toys. Infants looked longer at the single toy than the two toys. This finding shows that the infants were able to keep track of the two objects in their minds, even when the objects were hidden, and were surprised that one of the toys had disappeared. It also suggests that infants do not need to learn that objects do not spontaneously disappear. Related experiments by American developmental psychologist Renee Baillargeon show that without being taught, infants understand that objects cannot spontaneously appear, break apart, coalesce, or change size, shape, pattern, or color. This findings illustrate one of the basic building blocks of all cultures, the principle of persistence, which states that certain object changes are impossible.
Another type of thinking that may constrain cultures involves ideas about religion. American anthropologist and psychologist Pascal Boyer notes that religions share many similar beliefs across cultures—for example, the belief that something nonphysical, such as an invisible spirit, survives after a person’s death and can be contacted by a select few individuals. These ideas arise from basic beliefs shared among cultures about physics, biology, and the mind.
How Culture Shapes Thinking
Differences between cultures lead to different ways of thinking. Consider the differences between individualistic and collectivistic cultures. In an individualistic culture, people view themselves more as individuals and are taught to act independently, taking personal responsibility for their successes and failures. In a collectivistic culture, people view themselves more as members of groups and are taught to act interdependently, favoring the needs of the group over their own individual needs. The United States is an example of an individualistic culture, while most East Asian cultures are collectivistic.
These different cultural perspectives affect thinking in many ways. For example, one important finding in social psychology is the fundamental attribution error, which is the tendency to overestimate how much a person’s behavior is due to dispositional factors and to underestimate how much it is due to situational factors. Dispositional factors refer to a person’s internal characteristics, such as personality traits, abilities, and motives; situational factors refer to external causes. For example, students might explain that they did well on an exam because they are intelligenta dispositional factoror because the teacher gave an easy exama situational factor. Collectivistic cultures are less likely to make the fundamental attribution error than individualistic cultures. For example, cultural psychologists Michael Morris and Kaiping Peng examined newspaper reports of two mass murders and found that an American newspaper was more likely to describe the mass murders in terms of dispositional factors, such as a very bad temper, whereas a Chinese newspaper was more likely to focus on situational factors, such as isolation from the Chinese community due to having been recently fired.
Cultures arise in different geographical regions and environments, which can lead to important differences between cultures. One area of differences is the family structure. Most families across societies have parents who are monogamousone man married to one woman. However, in some families, there is polygamy, which includes polyandryone woman married to more than one manand polygynyone man married to more than one woman. According to American social psychologists Douglas Kenrick, Steven Neuberg, and Robert Cialdini, polygamy arises in cultures because of survival needs. For example, a polyandrous woman in Tibet may marry several men who are brothers because the harsh environment in the high Himalayan desert makes it difficult for a single man and woman to survive. The brothers in turn share the wife so that they can preserve the family estate from generation to generation. This family structure, which is called fraternal polyandry, appears to be driven by economic conditions originating from the environment.
Bibliography
Berry, John W., et al. Cross-Cultural Psychology: Research and Applications. 3rd ed. Cambridge University Print, 2011.
Cherry, Kendra. "What Is Cross-Cultural Psychology?" VeryWellMind, 16 Dec. 2023, www.verywellmind.com/what-is-cross-cultural-psychology-2794903. Accessed 28 Oct. 2024.
"Cross-Cultural Psychology." Psychology Today, 2024, www.psychologytoday.com/us/basics/cross-cultural-psychology. Accessed 28 Oct. 2024.
Goldstein, Susan. Cross-Cultural Explorations: Activities in Culture and Psychology. 2nd ed. Routledge, 2015.
Heine, Steven J. Cultural Psychology. 3d ed. Norton, 2016.
Keith, Kenneth D., ed. Cross-Cultural Psychology: Contemporary Themes and Perspectives. Wiley, 2011.
Krumov, Krum, and Knud S. Larsen. Cross-Cultural Psychology: Why Culture Matters. Information Age, 2013.
Laungani, Pittu D. Understanding Cross-Cultural Psychology: Eastern and Western Perspectives. Sage, 2007.
Matsumoto, David, and Fons J. R. van de Vijver, eds. Cross-Cultural Research Methods in Psychology. Cambridge University Press, 2011.
Ozer, Simon, Vladimer Lado Gamsakhurdia, and Seth J. Schwartz. "Investigating the Complexity of Cultural Identities in Globalizing Societies." Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 24 Oct. 2024, orcid.org/0000-0002-5297-4777. Accessed 28 Oct. 2024.
Nisbett, Richard E. The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently . . . and Why. 2003. Brealey, 2011.
Shiraev, Eric B., and David A. Levy. Cross-Cultural Psychology: Critical Thinking and Contemporary Applications. 5th ed. Pearson, 2013.